Carollia perspicillata

The vocal repertoire of captive Carollia perspicillata has been initially described by Fran Porter (Porter 1979). We contributed to Porter’s vocal repertoire description by analyzing additional vocalization types and by investigating the occurrence of vocal signatures in pup isolation calls and male courtship trills (Knörnschild et al. 2013 and 2014). Moreover, Ahana Fernandez and Nicolas Fasel from the University of Bern, Switzerland, analyzed vocal signatures in aggressive vocalizations from male C. perspicillata (Fernandez et al. 2014).

C perspicillata (Brinklov)   Carollia perspicillata (© Brinkløv).

 

Echolocation calls

Echolocation calls are used for orientation and foraging (Knörnschild et al. 2014).

C perspicillata_echolocation callsEcholocation calls of C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

 

Courtship trills

Harem males produce series of courtship trills when courting estrus females in their territories. Male courtship trills encode a distinct individual signature that probably faciliates female choice, mate recognition or neighbour-stranger recognition among male competitors (Knörnschild et al. 2014). Courtship trills are accompanied by stereotypic tactile and visual displays, such as hovering, wing flapping and poking (Knörnschild et al. 2014).

C perspicillata_courtship trillsCourtship trills of C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

 

 

Aggressive vocalizations

Both sexes produce down-sweeps during mildly aggressive encounters and warbles when aggression escalates. Moreover, harem males produce aggressive trills when defending their territory against male competitors (Knörnschild et al. 2014). Aggressive trills encode an individual signature that enables competitors to discriminate between male conspecifics (Fernandez et al. 2014). Male territorial defense comprises different and multimodal behavioral displays that are performed in a ritualized sequence (Fernandez et al. 2014).

C perspicillata_aggressive downsweepsDown-sweeps of C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

 C perspicillata_aggressive warblesAggressive warbles of C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

 

C perspicillata_aggressive trills

 Aggressive trills of male C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

 

 

Distress calls

Both sexes produce distress calls if they are the inferior part in a physical conflict (Knörnschild et al. 2014).

C perspicillata_distress calls

Distress calls of C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

 

 

Isolation calls

Pups produce isolation calls to solicit maternal care. Isolation calls encode a distinct individual signature. Mothers are able to discriminate between own and alien pups based on isolation calls alone (Knörnschild et al. 2013).

C perspicillata_isolation calls

Isolation calls of two C. perspicillata pups (© Knörnschild).

 

 

Wobbles

Both sexes produce wobbles in benign interactions, e.g. when several conspecifics are roosting close together and appear relaxed (Knörnschild et al. 2014).

C perspicillata_benign wobbles

Wobbles of C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

 

 

Other vocalization types

The behavioral context for three other vocalization types, namely V-shaped calls, flat down-sweeps and hooks, could not be determined (Knörnschild et al. 2014).

C perspicillata_V-shaped calls, flat downsweeps, hooks

V-shaped calls, flat down-sweeps, and hooks of C. perspicillata (© Knörnschild).

V-shaped calls

Flat down-sweeps

Hooks

 

Sonograms depict frequency as a function of time (1,024-point fast Fourier transform, 100% frame size, Hamming window with 87.5% overlap).